Appel à contributions prolongé : “Grammatical gender in diachrony / Le genre grammatical en diachronie”

Appel à contributions prolongé : “Grammatical gender in diachrony / Le genre grammatical en diachronie”

Atelier : Grammatical gender in diachrony / Le genre grammatical en diachronie

Délai prolongé jusqu’au 30 avril

Organisateurs :

Neige Rochant – neige.rochant@unil.ch – Université de Lausanne

• Marc Allassonnière-Tang – marc.allassonniere-tang@mnhn.fr – CNRS – Museum d’Histoire Naturelle

Colloque : Langues & Langage à la croisée des Disciplines,

1ère Rencontre annuelle LLcD, 9-14 septembre 2024, Sorbonne Université – Paris, France

Plus d’information : https://llcd2024.sciencesconf.org/

Envoi des abstracts en français ou en anglais à l’adresse :
https://easychair.org/conferences/?conf=llcd2024

Workshop description:

Languages can rely on various strategies to categorize nouns in the lexicon (Seifart 2010; Kemmerer 2017a). Grammatical gender, whose definition used for this workshop encompasses noun class systems, is one of the most common of these strategies (Corbett 2007). In gender systems, each noun of the lexicon is assigned to a specific category manifested by grammatical agreement patterns (Corbett 1991), with gender marking on elements associated with the noun in the noun phrase and/or the verb phrase, e.g. on the adjectives or verbs. The term ‘noun class’ tends to be used for languages with a larger set of categories in which other semantic features (such as humanness, shape, or plants) are primary.

Grammatical gender has attracted much attention from multiple areas of linguistics, e.g., typology (for its distribution and structural variation: Corbett 1991; Aikhenvald 2000; Grinevald 2000; Kilarski 2014), psycholinguistics (for the cognitive structures supporting it: Contini-Morava
& Kilarski 2013: 291–293), neuroscience (for how it is processed: Kemmerer 2017a,b), sociolinguistics (for how it reflects sociocultural gender: Hall 2002; Aikhenvald 2016a). This interest may be attributed to the wealth of information grammatical gender provides regarding human cognition and cultures by reflecting how the human brain categorizes its experience. This sphere of knowledge can be supplemented by the study of the evolution of gender systems, which is all the more interesting as the latter are held to result from long evolutionary chains and are in this sense “among the more clearly mature elements of language” (Dahl 2004).

Although gender systems of the world have been extensively studied in synchrony, the origins and details of their development remain highly hypothetical. The questions that still may be asked for a given subset of related languages pertain to multiple aspects of the evolution of gender:

Locus of gender-marking, e.g.: where was gender first marked and how did it spread to other parts of speech? Where is gender marked with higher diachronic stability?
Inventory of gender categories, e.g.: which gender categories are older, which have developed later on and how?
Gender assignment, e.g.: how and why does a lexeme change its gender? How did a gender become dominant?
Some of these questions have been investigated for the Indo-European family and several of its branches (Carling & Van Epps 2019; Carling et al. 2021; Allassonnière-Tang & Dunn 2020), among others. It is generally held, for example, that the feminine gender emerged from an original two-gender (animate/inanimate) system in early Indo-European (Luraghi 2011). Besides, typological hypotheses on the development of gender systems in general have been made. For instance, the most frequent trajectory involves several-stage grammaticalization from lexical nouns that develop into classifiers (Grinevald 2002; Aikhenvald 2016b). From then on, it has been proposed that classifiers can be repeated within the noun phrase or beyond, which gives rise to agreement via intermediate stages (Corbett 1991: 310–312; Givón 1976), e.g. as a result of the recruitment of classifying demonstrative pronouns as third-person personal pronouns (Greenberg 1978). Hence, gender typically starts from within the noun phrase (Tang & Her 2019). On the other hand, Luraghi (2011) established a correlation between the way a gender system arises and its primary function, arguing that systems born from the grammaticalization of classifiers fulfill a classificatory function and are primarily non-sex based, whereas those performing a referent-tracking function are born from the establishment of agreement following different morphosyntactic behavior of groups of nouns and are often sex-based. As regards the development of the motivations for gender assignment, it is believed that all gender systems start out as semantics-based (Audring 2016). Furthemore, the enrichment and
reduction of gender systems has been shown to proceed in cross-linguistically predictable ways (Demuth et al. 1986; Marchese 1988; Priestly 1983), with the birth of new agreement targets frequently resulting from grammaticalization, new gender values arising by reanalysis of existing morphological markers (Corbett 1991: 313–314) or being lost by syncretism or loss of the markers (very common in Indo-European, e.g. Polinsky & Van Everbroeck 2003) while distinctions are typically retained longest on personal pronouns (Corbett 1991: 143) and, in languages that lost grammatical agreement, gender markers generally still being found on nouns (Kießling 2018). Finally, the role of intra- and extra-linguistic factors in the evolution of gender systems has been little researched. Seifart (2018) proposed that semantically more opaque classification is less likely to diffuse through language contact. In addition, Allassonnière-Tang et al. (2021) showed that gender systems spread more by historical language expansion than classifier systems, which spread more by feature diffusion because they are less grammaticalized.

The evolution of gender in families other than Indo-European is less researched, though many hypotheses of development have been put forward for several families. For instance, Plaster & Polinsky 2007 proposed that gender developed from earlier noun classifier systems in several languages of Australia. Among the rare studies involving quantitative methods, Di Garbo & Verkerk (2022), a typological investigation of northwestern Bantu gender, suggests that animacy-based agreement contributes to the erosion of gender-marking and spreads in ways that are suggestive of a hierarchy of syntactic integration between nouns and adnominal modifiers. Another study by Rochant, Allassonnière-Tang & Cathcart (2022) investigated the evolutionary trends of noun class marking in Atlantic (Niger-Congo) using phylogenetic comparative methods.

This workshop aims to foster discussion on the diachrony of gender (including so-called ‘noun class’) systems in language families and/or areas from a typological perspective. It intends to bring together perspectives from different language families (including and beyond Indo-European) and different approaches, whether qualitative or quantitative.

References

Aikhenvald, Alexandra Y. (2000). Classifiers: A typology of noun categorization devices. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Aikhenvald, Alexandra Y (2016a). “Gender, shape, and sociality: How humans are special in Manambu”. In: International Journal of Language and Culture 3(1), 68–89.

Aikhenvald, Alexandra Y. (2016b). How gender shapes the world. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Aikhenvald, Alexandra Y. & Elena I. Mihas (2019). Genders and Classifiers: A Cross-Linguistic Typology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/oso/9780198842019.001.0001. url:
https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198842019.001.0001.

Allassonnière-Tang, Marc, Olof Lundgren, Maja Robbers, Sandra Cronhamn, Filip Larsson, One-Soon Her, Harald Hammarström, & Gerd Carling (2021). “Expansion
by migration and diffusion by contact is a source to the global diversity of linguistic nominal categorization systems”. In: Humanities and Social Sciences Communications 8(1), 1–6.

Allassonnière-Tang, Marc & Michael Dunn (2020). “The evolutionary trends of grammatical gender in Indo-Aryan languages”. In: Language Dynamics and Change Advance articles, 1–30. doi: 10.1163/22105832-bja10011.

Audring, Jenny (July 2016). “Gender”. In: Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.43.

Carling, Gerd & Briana Van Epps (2019). “Evolutionary aspects of Indo-European gender assign- ment”. In: 52nd Annual Meeting of the Societas Linguistica
Europaea (Book of Abstracts), 65. url: https://portal.research.lu.se/en/publications/evolutionary-aspects-of-indo-european-gender-assignment.

Carling, Gerd, Marc Allassonnière-Tang, Silvia Luraghi, Sunny Pydugadu, Olof Lundgren, & Filip Larsson (2021). “The lexical and typological trajectory of Indo-European gender evolution”. In: 54th Annual Meeting of the Societas Linguistica Europaea (Book of Abstracts). url: https:// portal.research.lu.se/en/publications/the-lexical-and-typological-trajectory-of-indo-european-gender-ev.

Contini-Morava, Ellen & Marcin Kilarski (2013). “Functions of nominal classification”. In: Language Sciences 40, 263–299. doi: 10.1016/j.langsci.2013.03.002.

Corbett, Greville G (1991). Gender. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Corbett, Greville G (2007). “Gender and noun classes”. In: Language typology and syntactic description. Vol. 3: Grammatical Categories and the Lexicon. Ed. by Timothy Shopen. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 241–279.

Demuth, Katherine, Nicholas Faraclas, & Lynell Marchese (1986). “Niger-Congo noun class and agreement systems in language acquisition and historical change”. In: Noun classes and categorization, 453–471.

Di Garbo, Francesca & Annemarie Verkerk (2022). “A typology of northwestern Bantu gender  systems”. In: Linguistics 60(4), 1169–1239.

Givón (1976). “Topic, Pronoun and Grammatical Agreement in Subject and Topic”. In: Subject and topic. Ed. by Charles N. Li. New York: Academic, 149–188.

Grinevald, Colette (2000). “A morphosyntactic typology of classifiers”. In: Systems of nominal classification. Ed. by Gunter Senft. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 50–92.

Grinevald, Colette (2002). “Making sense of nominal classification systems: Noun classifiers and the grammaticalization variable”. In: New reflections on grammaticalization. Ed. by Ilse Wischer & Gabrielle Diewald. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 259–275.

Hall, Kira (2002). “Unnatural gender in Hindi”. In: Gender across Languages: The Linguistic Representation of Women and Men. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 133–162.

Kemmerer, David (2017a). “Categories of object concepts across languages and brains: the relevance of nominal classification systems to cognitive neuroscience”. In: Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 32(4), 401–424. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1198819.

Kemmerer, David (2017b). “Some issues involving the relevance of nominal classification systems to cognitive neuroscience: response to commentators”. In: Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 32(4), 447–456. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1274414. url: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/23273798.2016.1274414.

Kießling, Roland (2018). “Niger-Congo numeral classifiers in a diachronic perspective”. In: Current Issues in Linguistic Theory 342: The Diachrony of Classification Systems. Ed. by William B. McGregor & Søren Wichmann, 33–76.

Kilarski, Marcin (2014). “The Place of Classifiers in the History of Linguistics”. In: Historiographia Linguistica 41(1), 33–79. doi: 10.1075/hl.41.1.02kil.

Luraghi, Silvia (2011). “The origin of the Proto-Indo-European gender system: Typological considerations”. In: Folia Linguistica 45(2), 435–464. doi: doi:10.1515/flin.2011.016.

Marchese, Lynell (1988). “Noun classes and agreement systems in Kru: A historical approach”. In: Agreement in natural languages: Approaches, theory, descriptions. Ed. by Barlow Michael & Ferguson Charles A. Stanford, CA: Center for the Study of Language and Information, 323–341.

McLaughlin, Fiona (1997). “Noun classification in Wolof when affixes are not renewed”. In: Studies in African linguistics 26(1), 1–28.

Münkemüller, Tamara, Sébastien Lavergne, Bruno Bzeznik, Stéphane Dray, Thibaut Jombart, Katja Schiffers, & Wilfried Thuiller (2012). “How to measure and test phylogenetic signal”. In: Methods in Ecology and Evolution 3(4), 743–756.

Plaster, Keith & Maria Polinsky (2007). Women are not dangerous things: Gender and categorization. Harvard Working Papers in Linguistics 12. 1–44.

Polinsky, Maria & Ezra Van Everbroeck (2003). “Development of gender classifications: Modeling the historical change from Latin to French”. In: Language 79(2), 356–390.

Priestly, Tom M. S. (1983). “On drift in Indo-European gender systems”. In: Journal of Indo- European Studies 11(3-4), 339–363.

Rochant, Neige, Marc Allassonnière-Tang, & Chundra Cathcart (2022). “The evolutionary trends of noun class systems in Atlantic languages”. In: Proceedings of the Joint Conference on Language Evolution (JCoLE). Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. Nijmegen, 624–631.

Seifart, Frank (2018). “The semantic reduction of the noun universe and the diachrony of nominal classification”. In: Current Issues in Linguistic Theory 342: The Diachrony of Classification Systems. Ed. by William B. McGregor & Søren Wichmann, 9–32.

Tang, Marc & One-Soon Her (2019). “Insights on the Greenberg-Sanches-Slobin generalization:Quantitative typological data on classifiers and plural markers”. In: Folia Linguistica 53(2), 297–331.